Unraveling the hidden link: neuroticism as a risk factor for upper urinary tract calculi—insights from Mendelian randomization analysis
Highlight box
Key findings
• The study revealed a causal relationship between neuroticism and upper urinary tract calculi (UUTC) and identified mood swings as an independent risk factor, with Mendelian randomization (MR) analysis. Significant associations were found with feelings of loneliness and worry. Results were robust, with no evidence of heterogeneity or pleiotropy, emphasizing the need for increased urinary stone surveillance in individuals with neurotic traits.
What is known and what is new?
• UUTC are significant risk factors for renal insufficiency and nephrectomy. Patients with UUTC often experience psychological distress, including heightened levels of depression and anxiety, which are frequently associated with neuroticism. However, there is limited research specifically linking neuroticism to UUTC.
• This study employs MR analysis to establish a causal relationship between neuroticism and UUTC. The findings suggested that neuroticism was a risk factor for UUTC, with specific traits such as experiencing mood swings identified as independent risk factors. These results contributed new insights into the connection between psychological factors and urological disorders, highlighting the need for increased surveillance of urinary stone disorders in individuals displaying neurotic traits.
What is the implication, and what should change now?
• The findings stress the need for enhanced surveillance of UUTC in individuals with neurotic tendencies, particularly in those characterized by mood swings.
Introduction
Upper urinary tract calculi (UUTC) and loss of follow-up are identified as the predominant risk factors for the development of renal insufficiency and the leading non-neoplastic cause of nephrectomy (1). A higher percentage of patients with urolithiasis suffer from psychological distress, including depression and anxiety (2). Depression is linked to an increased risk of developing kidney stones (3). Depression and anxiety are both marked by heightened neuroticism (4). Furthermore, personality traits, among which neuroticism is included, predict the occurrence of depression over the course of a person’s life (4). Research has identified a correlation between neuroticism and various urological disorders, including urinary incontinence, complicated urinary tract infections and cystitis (5-8). However, there is limited research on the relationship between UUTC and neuroticism. Therefore, it is crucial to investigate whether neuroticism can serve as a predictive factor for UUTC.
Conventional observational studies are susceptible to confounding variables, reversed causality, and various biases, leading to unreliable establishment of causal relationships. Mendelian randomization (MR) relies on the random assortment of alleles during the formation of gametes (9,10). As a result, genetic variations are spread throughout the population, typically unaffected by behavioral and environmental factors that may obscure the epidemiological connection between hypothetical risk factors and diseases. (9,10). In specific cases, this can offer research designs that closely mimic randomized comparisons (9,10).
Consequently, our objective was to examine the possible causal relationship between neuroticism and UUTC through MR analysis. We present this article in accordance with the STROBE-MR reporting checklist (available at https://tau.amegroups.com/article/view/10.21037/tau-24-379/rc).
Methods
Study design
We performed two-sample MR studies analysis to explore the causal relationship between neuroticism, as the exposure, and UTCC, as the outcome. Subsequently, we incorporated 11 neuroticism-related phenotypes for a subgroup analysis, which included irritable mood, feeling lonely, feeling miserable, experiencing mood swings, feeling guilty, worry too long after an embarrassing experience, feeling fed-up, feeling nervous, feeling worry, feeling hurt and feeling tense. The choice of SNPs [single nucleotide polymorphisms, refer to variations in a DNA sequence where a single nucleotide (A, T, C, or G) is altered, and this variation occurs in at least 1% of the population. SNPs are the most common type of genetic variation in the human genome]. as instrumental variables (IVs) followed three assumptions (11): (I) SNPs demonstrated strong correlations with neuroticism and 11 associated phenotypes; (II) SNPs should not be linked to any confounding factors [including body mass index (BMI), defined by calculating the ratio of weight to height, and diabetes]; and (III) there should be no direct genetic link between the SNPs and UUTC. Additionally, we conducted a multivariate MR analysis on phenotypes from the results of the two-sample analysis of 11 neuroticism-related phenotypes, which were significant, consistent in the direction of test methods, and free from pleiotropy, to explore the independent risk factors for UUTC. Additionally, we opted to use Neale Lab’s neuroticism score (ukb-a-230) for verification queue of the exposure (Figure 1).
Data source
We obtained genome-wide association study (GWAS) (a research method used to identify genetic variants associated with specific traits or diseases) data for neuroticism, neuroticism score and 11 associated traits, as well as diabetes and BMI, from the website: https://gwas.mrcieu.ac.uk/. For GWAS data on UUTC, we sourced it from the Finnish database version 10 (12) (Table 1). Given that our exposure and outcome data come from cohort studies conducted in different countries, there is no overlap in the sample sizes between the two. For each exposure, outcome, and other relevant variables, the methods of assessment and diagnostic criteria for diseases can be found in the references in Table 1. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (as revised in 2013).
Table 1
Phenotypes | GWAS ID | Population | Author | Sample size |
---|---|---|---|---|
Neuroticism | ebi-a-GCST005232 | European | Luciano M (13) | 329,821 |
Neuroticism score | ukb-a-230 | European | Neale | 274,108 |
Irritable mood | ebi-a-GCST006941 | European | Nagel M (14) | 366,726 |
Feeling lonely | ebi-a-GCST006942 | European | Nagel M (14) | 376,352 |
Feeling miserable | ebi-a-GCST006943 | European | Nagel M (14) | 376,097 |
Experiencing mood swings | ebi-a-GCST006944 | European | Nagel M (14) | 373,733 |
Feeling guilty | ebi-a-GCST006945 | European | Nagel M (14) | 373,380 |
Worry too long after an embarrassing experience | ebi-a-GCST006946 | European | Nagel M (14) | 367,725 |
Feeling fed-up | ebi-a-GCST006947 | European | Nagel M (14) | 374,971 |
Feeling nervous | ebi-a-GCST006948 | European | Nagel M (14) | 373,121 |
Feeling worry | ebi-a-GCST006950 | European | Nagel M (14) | 372,869 |
Feeling hurt | ebi-a-GCST006951 | European | Nagel M (14) | 372,047 |
Feeling tense | ebi-a-GCST006952 | European | Nagel M (14) | 371,318 |
UUTC | N14_CALCUKIDUR | European | Kurki MI (12) | 411,237 |
Diabetes | ebi-a-GCST90038633 | European | Dönertaş HM (15) | 484,598 |
BMI | ieu-b-40 | European | Yengo L (16) | 681,275 |
GWAS, genome-wide association study; ID, identifier; UUTC, upper urinary tract calculi; BMI, body mass index.
Instrument variable selection
The criteria for selecting IVs to assess the impact of exposures on outcome in our study were as follows: (I) SNPs associated with exposures that met the locus-wide significance threshold (P<5.0×10−8) were deemed potential IVs. (II) To counteract the influence of linkage disequilibrium (LD), we used the clumping method to identify independent SNPs, requiring an r2<0.001 and a minimum distance of more than 10,000 kb. (III) We systematically excluded intermediate allele palindromic SNPs from our analysis. (IV) We searched for SNPs related to diabetes and BMI on the LDlink (https://ldlink.nci.nih.gov/?tab=ldtrait) website and removed them. (V) The F-statistic was calculated as F = [R2 × (n − k − 1)]/[k × (1 − R2)], with R2 = 2 × (1 − MAF) × MAF × (beta/SD)2. Here, n represents the sample size of the exposure GWAS study, k was the number of SNPs, MAF was the minor allele frequency (similar to effect-allele frequency in R2 calculation), beta was the effect size of the SNP on exposure, and SD was the standard deviation (17). An F-value above 10 was commonly considered as a sign of strong IVs.
Statistical analysis
In our two-sample MR analysis, we took advantage of inverse variance weighted (IVW) (18,19), MR-Egger (20,21), and weighted median (22) methods. We chose the IVW method as our main analytical approach because of its strength in establishing causal effects.
We performed sensitivity analyses to evaluate the heterogeneity and pleiotropy, ensuring the robustness of our results from the two-sample MR analysis. Cochran’s Q test was utilized to detect heterogeneity among the SNPs, with a P value below 0.05 indicating the presence of heterogeneity (23,24). The MR-Egger intercept (20) and the MR-pleiotropy residual sum and outlier (PRESSO) method (25) were used to detect pleiotropy. P value below 0.05 was considered indicative of pleiotropy. We also employed MR-PRESSO to identify any outliers. When pleiotropy was not present, the IVW analysis was considered unbiased. To explore the independent influence of neuroticism phenotypes on UUTC, we conducted a multivariate MR analysis on phenotypes that demonstrated unbiased results in the IVW, with IVW P values below 0.05 and consistent directions across all three MR analysis methods (26,27).
For our MR analysis and follow-up sensitivity evaluations, we utilized Rstudio (version 4.3.2) in conjunction with the TwoSampleMR R package (version 0.5.10).
Results
The reported results are based on the IVs after removing outliers. MR-PRESSO identified outliers only in the phenotypes neuroticism score, Feeling nervous, Feeling worry, and Feeling tense (Table S1).
Causal impact of neuroticism on UUTC
The findings from IVW [odds ratio (OR) =1.15, 95% confidence interval (CI): 1.02 to 1.29, P=0.03] and weighted median (OR =1.22, 95% CI: 1.03 to 1.44, P=0.02) analyses suggested that neuroticism was a significant factor in UUTC, with a statistically significant association (P<0.05). MR-Egger estimate also displayed their positive effect on the UUTC. Verification queue similarly confirmed that the neuroticism score was a risk factor for UUTC (IVW, OR =1.11, 95% CI: 1.03 to 1.19, P=0.007). The sensitivity analysis showed that the Q values for the Cochran’s Q test of the IVW and MR-Egger methods were both 53.40, with P values of 0.46 and 0.42. These findings suggested no presence of heterogeneity. The MR-Egger intercept (P=0.94) does not support the presence of pleiotropy (Figure 2). Furthermore, the MR-PRESSO analysis demonstrates a global test P value of 0.50, indicating the absence of pleiotropic effects (Table 2).
Table 2
Exposure | Global test RSSobs | Global test P value |
---|---|---|
Neuroticism | 55.24939 | 0.50 |
Neuroticism score | 58.26299518 | 0.33 |
Experiencing mood swings | 33.06842427 | 0.58 |
Feeling lonely | 5.820765 | 0.44 |
Feeling worry | 31.43784 | 0.32 |
Worry too long after an embarrassing experience | 13.50418309 | 0.43 |
Feeling miserable | 20.65213 | 0.75 |
MR-PRESSO, Mendelian randomization pleiotropy residual sum and outlier; RSSobs, the observed residual sum of squares.
Causal impact of 11 neuroticism-related phenotypes on UUTC
Following two-sample MR analysis of 11 neuroticism-related traits and UUTC, we identified 5 phenotypes that met the criteria for unbiased results in the IVW and consistent directions across all three MR analysis methods. All results are available in Table S2. All MR-PRESSO results are in Table S3.
The IVW results reveal significant positive associations with UUTC for experiencing mood swings (OR =1.41, 95% CI: 1.01 to 1.97, P=0.047), feeling lonely (OR =6.03, 95% CI: 2.12 to 17.10, P<0.001), and feeling worry (OR =1.58, 95% CI: 1.04 to 2.38, P=0.03), all with P values below 0.05. Feeling miserable showed a negative correlation with UUTC across three MR analysis methods, but the results are not statistically significant (IVW, P=0.44; MR-Egger, P=0.74; weighted median, P=0.36). Three MR analysis methods suggested a positive correlation between worry too long after an embarrassing experience and UUTC, though the findings lack statistical significance (IVW, P=0.35; MR-Egger, P=0.24; weighted median, P=0.25). The Q-test and MR-Egger results for the five aforementioned phenotypes revealed no evidence of heterogeneity or pleiotropy (Figure 2). The MR-PRESSO test results also do not support pleiotropy (Table 2).
Multivariate MR analysis
In our study, we conducted a multivariable MR analysis on the three significant phenotypes identified in section 3.2—experiencing mood swings, feeling lonely, and feeling worry—along with diabetes and BMI, to determine which of these behaviors independently contributed to the risk of UUTC. We found that the phenotype “feeling lonely” did not yield any SNPs; hence, we conducted a multivariable analysis on “experiencing mood swings”, “feeling worry”, diabetes, and BMI. Experiencing mood swings independently contributed (OR =1.75, 95% CI: 1.06 to 2.89, P=0.03) to the risk of UUTC (Figure 3).
Discussion
In the two-sample MR study, we examined the association between neuroticism, including 11 related personality traits, and UUTC. Our findings indicated that neuroticism, along with experiencing mood swings, feeling lonely, and feeling worry, act as risk factors for UUTC. Verification queue also supports that neuroticism score is a risk factor for UUTC. Sensitivity analyses showed no evidence of heterogeneity or pleiotropy. To investigate if experiencing mood swings, feeling lonely, and feeling worry are independent risk factors for UUTC, we performed a multivariable MR analysis while also considering diabetes and BMI. The results of the multivariable analysis indicated that experiencing mood swings is an independent risk factor for UUTC.
Hormonal and metabolism imbalances may be the primary causes through which neuroticism leads to UUTC. Studies have confirmed that individuals demonstrating high levels of neuroticism tend to have higher levels of cortisol (28,29). Cortisol may increase urinary calcium concentration (30), potentially through the inhibition of calcium reabsorption in the renal tubules (31). The risk of stone formation significantly increases with an elevated level of urinary calcium (32). According to research on high cortisol levels, even healthy children with higher cortisol levels within the normal range may have negative effects on the kidney’s citrate excretion (33). In adult males, low citrate in urine may commonly trigger the formation of calcium kidney stones (34). Citrate seems to prevent the crucial process of crystal agglomeration, where separate calcium oxalate crystals join to create a stone (35). Elevated neuroticism is linked to decreased vitamin D levels (36). Having high serum vitamin D levels may be connected to a lower risk of stone formation or recurrence (37). A potential mechanism for the heightened risk of UUTC in individuals with neuroticism could be disturbances in calcium-phosphate metabolism, which are secondary to their reduced vitamin D levels. Regarding the variations in cortisol and vitamin D levels across different seasons and among individuals with different neurotic traits, our study did not include specific analyses in this area due to a lack of corresponding stratified data. Cortisol levels may be influenced by circadian rhythms and stress factors, while vitamin D levels are related to sunlight exposure and exhibit seasonal changes. Future research could consider: (I) investigating the relationships between traits such as “experiencing mood swings” with physiological indicators (such as cortisol and vitamin D levels); (II) examining the impact of seasonal factors on these physiological indicators and their expression in populations with high neuroticism. This will help deepen our understanding of the pathways through which neuroticism affects the risk of UUTC, providing a basis for clinical interventions. In individuals with neuroticism, a heightened inflammatory state could potentially serve as an underlying contributor to the development of UUTC. Neuroticism in young adults is linked to higher levels of interleukin-6 (IL-6) levels (38). Oncostatin M, a member of the IL-6 family, may promote the formation of kidney crystal deposits by directly affecting renal tubular epithelial cells and renal fibroblasts to produce crystal-binding molecules and inflammatory cytokines (39). Unhealthy lifestyle choices could potentially serve as another significant factor. There is a positive correlation between sedentary behavior and neuroticism levels (40,41). Sedentary behavior is a risk factor for kidney stones (42). Neurotic personality traits are associated with the occurrence of cystitis, urinary leakage, and difficulty in urination (8). Although there is no direct evidence indicating that individuals with neurotic traits reduce fluid intake, these patients may decrease their water consumption to reduce urination frequency due to fear of the aforementioned lower urinary tract symptoms. Therefore, reduced fluid intake may play a mediating role between neuroticism and UUTC. Future research should further explore the impact of neuroticism on fluid intake behavior and whether this is a pathway through which neuroticism increases the risk of UUTC.
In our research, MR analysis emerges as a crucial technique, providing a robust framework for assessing causality and addressing the limitations inherent in traditional observational studies. By employing genetic variants as IVs, we significantly mitigated the risk of confounding factors and reverse causality, thereby strengthening the validity of the causal link between neuroticism and 11 related psychological traits and UUTC. Nevertheless, it is essential to acknowledge certain limitations. Primarily, our dependence on summary-level data from large-scale GWASs may introduce biases and inherit limitations from the original studies. Additionally, our findings are based on European populations, which limits their generalizability to other ethnic groups. Future research should aim to include diverse ethnic cohorts to validate our results. Lastly, due to the lack of specific databases, we were unable to conduct subgroup analyses on neuroticism and the composition of stones. Furthermore, considering the significant role of gender in the incidence of UUTC (43), future research should delve into whether gender differences modulate or influence the relationship between neuroticism and UUTC. This may involve stratified analyses to assess whether there are differences in the strength and patterns of this association among men and women, thereby providing more targeted prevention and intervention strategies. Given that our study utilized public databases, we did not find GWAS data on neuroticism and UUTC stratified by gender in known public databases. In the future, we will closely monitor updates to these databases to further explore the impact of gender stratification on the relationship between neuroticism and UUTC.
Given that UUTC are the primary non-neoplastic cause of nephrectomy (1), early identification and preventive measures in at-risk groups are crucial. This strategy can significantly alleviate patient suffering and offer economic benefits to healthcare systems. Our study results indicated that neuroticism is a risk factor for UUTC, with experiencing mood swings identified as an independent risk factor for UUTC. This underscores the importance of closely monitoring nephrolithiasis in neurotic individuals and contributes to our comprehension of the brain-kidney axis.
Conclusions
Our MR analysis has identified neuroticism as a risk factor for UUTC, with experiencing mood swings identified as an independent risk factor for UUTC, thereby deepening our understanding of the brain-kidney axis. The specific mechanisms underlying this association warrant further investigation. These findings underscore the importance of monitoring urinary stones in neurotic individuals.
Acknowledgments
We express our gratitude to the Medical Research Council Integrative Epidemiology Unit (MRC IEU) consortium, at the University of Bristol for developing the IEU open GWAS project and for extracting the GWAS data. We also wish to acknowledge the contribution of the participants and investigators of the FinnGen study.
Funding: This work was financially supported by
Footnote
Reporting Checklist: The authors have completed the STROBE-MR reporting checklist. Available at https://tau.amegroups.com/article/view/10.21037/tau-24-379/rc
Peer Review File: Available at https://tau.amegroups.com/article/view/10.21037/tau-24-379/prf
Conflicts of Interest: Both authors have completed the ICMJE uniform disclosure form (available at https://tau.amegroups.com/article/view/10.21037/tau-24-379/coif). The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
Ethical Statement: The authors are accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (as revised in 2013).
Open Access Statement: This is an Open Access article distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0), which permits the non-commercial replication and distribution of the article with the strict proviso that no changes or edits are made and the original work is properly cited (including links to both the formal publication through the relevant DOI and the license). See: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
References
- Mao S, Jiang H, Wu Z, et al. Urolithiasis: the most risk for nephrectomy in nonrenal tumor patients. J Endourol 2012;26:1356-60. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Ní Néill E, Richards HL, Hennessey D, et al. Psychological Distress in Patients With Urolithiasis: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. J Urol 2023;209:58-70. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Wang M, Jian Z, Ma Y, et al. Depression increases the risk of kidney stone: Results from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2007-2018 and Mendelian randomization analysis. J Affect Disord 2022;312:17-21. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Yang Z, Li A, Roske C, et al. Personality traits as predictors of depression across the lifespan. J Affect Disord 2024;356:274-83. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Yarnell JW, Voyle GJ, Sweetnam PM, et al. Factors associated with urinary incontinence in women. J Epidemiol Community Health 1982;36:58-63. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Terracciano A. Personality traits and the risk of urinary incontinence: Evidence from three longitudinal samples. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2024;39:e6084. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Hunt JC, Waller G. Psychological factors in recurrent uncomplicated urinary tract infection. Br J Urol 1992;69:460-4. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Zhang X, Ma L, Li J, et al. Mental health and lower urinary tract symptoms: Results from the NHANES and Mendelian randomization study. J Psychosom Res 2024;178:111599. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Smith GD, Ebrahim S. Mendelian randomization: prospects, potentials, and limitations. Int J Epidemiol 2004;33:30-42. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Smith GD, Ebrahim S. 'Mendelian randomization': can genetic epidemiology contribute to understanding environmental determinants of disease? Int J Epidemiol 2003;32:1-22. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Emdin CA, Khera AV, Kathiresan S. Mendelian Randomization. JAMA 2017;318:1925-6. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Kurki MI, Karjalainen J, Palta P, et al. FinnGen provides genetic insights from a well-phenotyped isolated population. Nature 2023;613:508-18. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Luciano M, Hagenaars SP, Davies G, et al. Association analysis in over 329,000 individuals identifies 116 independent variants influencing neuroticism. Nat Genet 2018;50:6-11. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Nagel M, Watanabe K, Stringer S, et al. Item-level analyses reveal genetic heterogeneity in neuroticism. Nat Commun 2018;9:905. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Dönertaş HM, Fabian DK, Valenzuela MF, et al. Common genetic associations between age-related diseases. Nat Aging 2021;1:400-12. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Yengo L, Sidorenko J, Kemper KE, et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for height and body mass index in ~700000 individuals of European ancestry. Hum Mol Genet 2018;27:3641-9. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Burgess S, Thompson SG. Avoiding bias from weak instruments in Mendelian randomization studies. Int J Epidemiol 2011;40:755-64. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Slob EAW, Burgess S. A comparison of robust Mendelian randomization methods using summary data. Genet Epidemiol 2020;44:313-29. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Burgess S, Scott RA, Timpson NJ, et al. Using published data in Mendelian randomization: a blueprint for efficient identification of causal risk factors. Eur J Epidemiol 2015;30:543-52. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Bowden J, Davey Smith G, Burgess S. Mendelian randomization with invalid instruments: effect estimation and bias detection through Egger regression. Int J Epidemiol 2015;44:512-25. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Burgess S, Thompson SG. Interpreting findings from Mendelian randomization using the MR-Egger method. Eur J Epidemiol 2017;32:377-89. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Bowden J, Davey Smith G, Haycock PC, et al. Consistent Estimation in Mendelian Randomization with Some Invalid Instruments Using a Weighted Median Estimator. Genet Epidemiol 2016;40:304-14. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Cohen JF, Chalumeau M, Cohen R, et al. Cochran's Q test was useful to assess heterogeneity in likelihood ratios in studies of diagnostic accuracy. J Clin Epidemiol 2015;68:299-306. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Greco M FD, Minelli C, Sheehan NA, et al. Detecting pleiotropy in Mendelian randomisation studies with summary data and a continuous outcome. Stat Med 2015;34:2926-40. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Verbanck M, Chen CY, Neale B, et al. Detection of widespread horizontal pleiotropy in causal relationships inferred from Mendelian randomization between complex traits and diseases. Nat Genet 2018;50:693-8. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Burgess S, Thompson SG. Multivariable Mendelian randomization: the use of pleiotropic genetic variants to estimate causal effects. Am J Epidemiol 2015;181:251-60. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Sanderson E, Davey Smith G, Windmeijer F, et al. An examination of multivariable Mendelian randomization in the single-sample and two-sample summary data settings. Int J Epidemiol 2019;48:713-27. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Garcia-Banda G, Chellew K, Fornes J, et al. Neuroticism and cortisol: pinning down an expected effect. Int J Psychophysiol 2014;91:132-8. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Nater UM, Hoppmann C, Klumb PL. Neuroticism and conscientiousness are associated with cortisol diurnal profiles in adults--role of positive and negative affect. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010;35:1573-7. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Cosman F, Nieves J, Herbert J, et al. High-dose glucocorticoids in multiple sclerosis patients exert direct effects on the kidney and skeleton. J Bone Miner Res 1994;9:1097-105. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Reid IR, Ibbertson HK. Evidence for decreased tubular reabsorption of calcium in glucocorticoid-treated asthmatics. Horm Res 1987;27:200-4. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Curhan GC, Taylor EN. 24-h uric acid excretion and the risk of kidney stones. Kidney Int 2008;73:489-96. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Hua Y, Esche J, Hartmann MF, et al. Cortisol and 11 beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2 as potential determinants of renal citrate excretion in healthy children. Endocrine 2020;67:442-8. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Rudman D, Kutner MH, Redd SC 2nd, et al. Hypocitraturia in calcium nephrolithiasis. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1982;55:1052-7. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Kok DJ, Papapoulos SE, Bijvoet OL. Crystal agglomeration is a major element in calcium oxalate urinary stone formation. Kidney Int 1990;37:51-6. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Luchetti M, et al. The mediating role of Vitamin D in the association between personality and memory: Evidence from two samples. Biol Psychol 2023;178:108525. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Li Z, Li L, Zheng J, et al. Associations between lumbar bone mineral density, serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D and history of kidney stones in adults aged 30-69 years in the USA (NHANES 2011-2018). BMJ Open 2023;13:e070555. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Syk M, Isaksson J, Rasmusson AJ, et al. Neuroticism is positively associated with leptin/adiponectin ratio, leptin and IL-6 in young adults. Sci Rep 2021;11:9690. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Yamashita S, Komori T, Kohjimoto Y, et al. Essential roles of oncostatin M receptor β signaling in renal crystal formation in mice. Sci Rep 2020;10:17150. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Allen MS, Walter EE, McDermott MS. Personality and sedentary behavior: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychol 2017;36:255-63. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Ebstrup JF, Aadahl M, Eplov LF, et al. Cross-sectional associations between the five factor personality traits and leisure-time sitting-time: the effect of general self-efficacy. J Phys Act Health 2013;10:572-80. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Liu M, Wu J, Gao M, et al. Lifestyle factors, serum parameters, metabolic comorbidities, and the risk of kidney stones: a Mendelian randomization study. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 2023;14:1240171. [Crossref] [PubMed]
- Scales CD Jr, Smith AC, Hanley JM, et al. Prevalence of kidney stones in the United States. Eur Urol 2012;62:160-5. [Crossref] [PubMed]